Physical Address
304 North Cardinal St.
Dorchester Center, MA 02124
Physical Address
304 North Cardinal St.
Dorchester Center, MA 02124
A new study published on January 21 in the journal Aggressive Behavior examines how “indirect aggression,” colloquially known as meanness, develops from childhood to adulthood.
The study followed 704 children for 12 years.
It found that the 9% who reported frequently being mean at age 10 were 9x to 14x more likely to report frequently being mean at age 22.
The other 91% – i.e. the children who already scored low or very low on the meanness scale at age 10 – generally became even less mean over time.
Researchers know a lot about kids’ use of physical (or “direct”) aggression, because its negative impacts are obvious.
But they know less about “indirect aggression,” also known as relational or social aggression.
Most people colloquially refer to this kind of aggression as being “mean.”
Typical examples include spreading hurtful gossip, excluding someone from a group, etc.
Most people consider this form of aggression more benign than hitting or scratching.
That is despite much research showing that meanness is anything but benign.
In fact, indirect aggression is associated with increased depression, anxiety, and even suicide in its targets.
It can also lead to negative outcomes for its perpetrators.
This gap in knowledge inspired University of Ottawa researchers Tracy Vaillancourt and Ann H. Farrell to investigate how indirect aggression typically develops from childhood onward.
The studies that already exist on this topic tend to follow children only until late adolescence.
But what happens to meanness when kids become adults?
To find out, Vaillancourt and Farrell began following students from 51 Ontario schools in 2008.
The students’ average age at the time was ten.
The researchers collected data on them for the next 12 years.
The study wound up including 704 students.
There were slightly more females than males.
The researchers measured the participants’ levels of indirect aggression using a variety of tools.
The tool they used to test the children from ages 10 – 18 was the Aggressive Behavior Scale.
On this assessment, participants have to respond to a series of statements on a scale of one to four (i.e. from “not true at all” to “completely true”).
Example items include “I’m the kind of person who puts others down,” “who says mean things to others,” and “who ignores others or stops talking to them.”
The tool the researchers used when the participants had become adults (i.e. aged 19 – 22) was the Indirect Aggression Scale.
This scale contained sample items such as “I have purposefully left others out of activities,” “made negative comments about others’ physical appearance,” “made fun of others in public,” and “made others feel that they don’t fit in.”
The researchers also collected information on the participants’ gender and ethnicity, as well as their parents’ income level and level of education.
Of course, these aggression measures are based on self-reports, which can be tainted by “self‐serving biases” such as the desire to present oneself in a positive light.
Peer reports from parents, teachers, or classmates might yield more accurate results.
But, as the study points out, there is no evidence that peer reports are any more accurate than self-reports of indirect aggression. “Indirect aggression becomes increasingly covert as children age,” the paper’s authors write, which means it cannot be as easily spotted by outsiders as physical aggression can.
For example, indirect aggression in early childhood might consist of a child saying “you can’t sit with me.” But in older children and adults, indirect aggression often takes on more indirect forms, for example deliberately not tagging someone in a social media post.
For children from 10 to 18 years old, the researchers discerned four main “trajectories.”
The most common one, which applied to about 65% of the sample, was the “very low decreasing” group.
Children in this group very rarely used indirect aggression, and their use of it decreased even further over time.
This group consisted of a roughly equal number of boys and girls.
The second-largest group, “low decreasing,” accounted for about 26% of the sample.
This group also consisted of a roughly equal number of girls and boys.
Children in this group used “some” indirect aggression in late childhood, but this use decreased by late adolescence.
The next group, “low to moderate increasing,” formed about 5% of the sample.
Their use of meanness started low, but steadily increased over time.
The final and smallest group, “moderate increasing,” accounted for about 4% of the total.
They “often” used indirect aggression from ages 10 to 18, and this use increased over time.
This group consisted of about three-quarters girls.
In other words, most children (91%) initially used little or very little meanness, and these already-low levels dropped even further over time.
The other 9% became increasingly mean from late childhood to late adolescence.
Looking at the young adult sample (aged 19 – 22), the researchers discerned two main trajectories.
One was a “low decreasing” group (83%) who started with low levels of meanness that dropped even further by age 22.
The other group was “moderate stable,” who used moderate amounts of meanness, and continued to do so over these four years.
In other words, there was a mean group, and a not-mean group.
The researchers found that children who were in either of the “increasing” groups during ages 10 – 18 were much more likely than the children in either of the “decreasing” groups to wind up in the “mean” group of adults.
Specifically, children who were on the “moderate increasing trajectory” from age 10 to 18 were nine times more likely wind up in the mean group when they were 22.
And those in the “low‐to‐moderate increasing” group were 14 times more likely to wind up there.
In other words, the mean kids generally wound up becoming mean adults.
Or, as the researchers put it, “these results highlight the strong continuity of indirect aggression over time.”
These findings differ from the typical trajectories of physical aggression.
Normally, almost all children (about 96%) eventually stop using physical aggression.
Hardly any show increasing levels from childhood to adolescence and adulthood.
“Even though most children are socialized out of using physical aggression,” Vaillancourt and Farrell write, “the same cannot be said about indirect aggression.”
In terms of limitations, this study took place in Canada, which is not generally known as an especially mean place.
As such, results in other locations might vary.
In terms of future directions, researchers could look into why some kids become less mean (or more mean) over time.
What are the factors that distinguish children who follow increasing meanness trajectories from those who follow decreasing trajectories.
Possible predictors that the current paper mentions are “high levels of maternal psychological control, exposure to indirect aggression in the media, and sibling hostility.”
It is also important to examine the “status-related costs and benefits” of indirect aggression.
Do the perpetrators get rewards for their meanness? Research has long shown that “aggression can serve a utilitarian purpose.”
In particular, research has indicated that people use indirect aggression to acquire or maintain social rewards. such as increased social status and power.
And they do so “with few peer‐based sanctions” compared to physical aggression.
Likewise, many programs already exist to reduce physical aggression in children.
But there are very few, if any, designed to reduce indirect aggression.
This gap should be closed, especially considering the “large body of evidence demonstrating that indirect aggression causes harm,” the researchers write.
“Given the negative impact indirect aggression has on others,” they conclude, “intervening early to derail this pattern of abuse is justified.”
Study: “Mean kids become mean adults: Trajectories of indirect aggression from age 10 to 22”
Authors: Tracy Vaillancourt and Ann H. Farrell
Published in: Aggressive Behavior
Publication date: January 21, 2021
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21950
Photo: by Diana via Pexels